(Proveit.txt) A HISTORY OF THE ORIGIN OF THE BOX-CAMERA THEORY OF THE EYE A QUESTION CONCERNING THE NATURE OF PROOF Reference: "Introduction to Physiological Optics", by James Southall I have often hear the words, "prove it", with respect to the fact that the fundamental eye can have a negative refractive state. Since the "it" is never described, it follows that no one can ever "prove it". But if the requirement is to prove that the eye is an auto-focused camera versus the box-camera concept, then proof is possible. I wrote my book so that you could help your own daughter avoid nearsightedness. I know you want to help others but -- regardless of proof -- I do not think the general public will understand what must be done. Dr. Jacob Raphaelson went through this 100 years ago, as described in, "The Printer's Son". The public wants their distant vision sharpened instantly, and expects this of you. Anything beyond that point they will reject -- unless they are very motivated and intelligent about this difficult situation. It is very clear that the person who desires prevention must have strong motivation and support from you if the person plans to use the plus lens effectively. In my opinion, your work with the public is very difficult because the public is not logical, and not consistent in what they expect. Unless they have the motivation, they will not push hard in the proper use of a plus lens. A professional pilot, who looks at his own eye chart and sees the results as they develop will be your best candidate for effective prevention. The public demands immediate results and does not listen to explanations. They will quit an effort if their is the slightest problem, or if some other ophthalmologist or optometrist uses "scare" tactics against them. If this happens, they will quit the effort and blame you for anything that might happen with their eyes. There is no incentive to attempt to help most people -- and both you and I understand that truth. The health profession has no choice except to apply a minus lens and (with a few exceptions) suggests that anyone who asks deeper questions about these issues must be "not-scientific". I can accept this as the reality of medicine. They should say "non-medical", rather than "not-scientific". In science, you pay attention to direct objective measurements. This is not quite true in optometry -- as I described above. Tragically, this unfortunate situation has continued since its inception 400 years ago. The theory of the eye began this way: 1. The lens-developers dealing with the public found a plus lens that would sharpen near vision -- when you reached old-age. 2. In addition, they found that young people with slightly blurry distant vision, could clear their distant vision with a minus lens. The theory of using a lens on the public is based on the above understanding of responsibility and resulting direct action. There has been very little improvement in this concept of the eye. Around 1600 Johan Kepler (Astronomer) began developing a pure-refractive theory of the eye. This was good work, but assumed that you could "freeze" the eye and make all your measurements based on the box-camera concept. This idea never attempted to analyze the eye's dynamic behavior at all -- only the refractive properties of an intellectually frozen eye. This analysis this approach was good, so the fact that the eye is not frozen was forgotten. Kepler's analysis could be used to support Items 1 and 2. For this reason the "frozen eye" theory was accepted as a medical theory -- and anyone who challenged the concept concerning the bad results of item 2 were told that Kepler's theory was "proven" and that the natural eye is a rigid box cameras. Kepler's theory was further refined and re-published in 1858 by two ophthalmologists, Dr. Donders and Dr. Helmholtz. They accepted Kepler's frozen-eye concept, and added further assumptions. 1. They assumed that a focal state of exactly zero could be considered normal. Donders invented the word emmetropia to describe this idealization of the "frozen" eye. 2. They assumed that any focal state other than exactly zero must be a defect, or "refractive error". They invented the word "ametropia" to describe both positive and negative focal states of all normal eyes. Don't get me wrong at this point. These were great men in medicine at that time. But they continued the academic assumption of Kepler, that you could "freeze" the eye and do a pure refractive analysis. They also assumed that you could translate a relative focal state into an absolute dimensions. (i.e., they assumed that if the eye had a focal state of zero, it must have an exact length of 24.38 mm. In fact no relationship has ever been established.) By doing this, they thought that they made the Kepler's theory into proof that the eye was "too long", when the natural eye simply had a normal but undesired negative focal state. This box-camera theory made the use of a plus or minus lens seem more systematic, although it requires a belief that the eye is defective if it has a negative or positive focal state. (i.e., if your eyes have a focal state that is not zero, you are suffering from "stress and strain" because the eye is too long or too short. The reasoning here is circular, because it is not proven that a focal state of zero corresponds to a exact length. It is only an assumption that you can convert relative measurements into absolute dimensions.) In any event, this theory makes all eyes defective by definition -- a thesis of doubtful validity. Why should we object to Kepler's theory, which became the a theory of practice? As a theory that allows refractive analysis of an idealized eye it is excellent. As a theory of the eye that reproduces the actual motion and change of focal state of the natural eye -- it is not accurate. Kepler's pure-refractive theory was correct, but the assumptions of the follow-on (Donders-Helmholtz) theory are not accurate or correct. In the light of experimental data developed in the last 50 years we should begin developing a better conceptual model of the eye's dynamic behavior. The experimental facts demonstrate that all eyes change their focal state as the visual environment is changed. By reference to the facts, the eye is established to be a well designed auto-focused camera. (i.e., you can make ALL eyes nearsighted by forced wearing of a minus lens.) The type of data needed to demonstrate this truth was not available in 1860. So the original conception should undergo evolution to account for these recently developed facts. But in fact, the operative reasons for using a plus or minus lens have not change since their original inception -- 400 years ago. Thus the "theory of the eye" is driven by expediency, and not by objective scientific facts -- in my humble opinion. Science is based on objective facts. We should be able to recognize that there is a problem with expanding Kepler's theory, beyond its original intended scope. He did an excellent refractive analysis. He did not intend that we believe that all eyes are rigid box cameras that are defective because they have focal states other than zero. We suggest that the natural eye is an auto-focused camera, and that, for this reason, the natural eyes must change its focal state (which you measure) as you change the visual environment (which you control). Since we are using neutral language to describe this situation, it follows that experimental conformation (that all eyes are auto-focused cameras) will be straight forward. The nature of this type of experiment can hardly be argued. This means that the evolution-designed eye can have both negative and positive focal states, and not be defective. In fact, the measured focal state of your eyes is directly dependent on your accommodation level -- in diopters. Obviously, if you work for long hours, your normal eyes are going to develop a negative focal state. This is perfectly normal and an expected for an auto-focus camera. The Helmholtz-Donders theory, and its required assumption has never been objectively tested -- as stated by Dr. William H. Bates. This means, that the box-camera picture of the eye is misleading at best. At worst, it blinds us to a potential method of preventing the development of nearsightedness by aggressive use of a plus lens. Sincerely, Otis Brown